THE
question of the powers and limitations of the
organs of sight in fish, and the extent to which they enable them to
discriminate between colours, is an interesting and difficult problem,
appealing strongly to the fisherman as well as to the naturalist.
As in all questions connected
with the investigation of the life-history of fish, the matter is rendered
all the more difficult by the fact of their inhabiting a different medium
from ourselves. Nevertheless the experienced angler who is also an observant
man,—and the two are not necessarily always synonymous,—must have often had
opportunity of noting facts throwing light on some aspects of the question.
From the point of view of the
fisherman the sense of sight of fishes, and particularly of the Salmonidae,
is doubtless of much importance. That their eyesight is acute, to an extent
that is proverbial, needs no demonstration. The veriest tyro, as he passes
along the banks of a clear trout-stream, will soon learn this elementary
fact for himself. As his education proceeds, he will also speedily note that
the trout's range of vision is chiefly forward and upward ; and lying, as
they necessarily do when at rest, with their heads up-stream, he finds that
to approach within reasonable distance unseen he must keep down-stream from
them and as low as possible. This is the foundation of the first principle
of clear-water stream fishing, wading upstream.
A much more intricate and
difficult question is that as to the power of discrimination of colour by
fish. Do they perceive the differences of colour; if so in what degree ; and
do these colours produce the same effect on the fish's as upon the human
eye? It is permissible to assume that as fishes see, partly, at least,
through a different medium, the resultant impressions received by them will
vary from that in our own experience. In the case of the angler's fly, it
will be generally seen by the fish against the sky, that is, against the
source of light; and if we hold a salmon-fly, however gaudy and brilliant,
between our own eyes and the sky, we find but a dark silhouette appearing,
with possibly reflected rays of coloured light from hackle-fibre or tinsel ;
but all the meticulous minutiae of the fly-dresser have disappeared.
Half a century ago the salmon
flies of the Tweed, and generally throughout Scotland, were sombre affairs,
as is seen by the patterns of Stoddart and others ; black, red or claret
rnohair or pigswool, a hackle dyed or plain, a turkey, mallard or teal wing,
with perhaps a little tinsel. But Stoddart thought it wiser that the tinsel,
if used, should be tarnished. When later the fashion of using the bright,
so-called Irish, flies came in, the solemn warning was given that the fish
were being frightened back into the sea.
To-day the gaudy fly reigns
almost indisputed. Yet there are many anglers, and these not the least
experienced, who hold that if the old quiet patterns were once more fished
as assiduously as the new the results would be much or quite the same; that
the 'best fly,' in fact, is still that which is oftenest and longest in the
water.
Formerly there were few
professional tackle-makers. Flies were dressed by the angler himself or
obtained from the local semi-professional who, in the days of unprotected
waters, was everywhere to be found. These naturally used the materials
readiest to hand, and the results being satisfactory, they were content. The
increase in the ranks of fishermen, consequent on increased travelling
facilities, changed all that, and much else. New men brought new ideas, new
materials, new experiments.
A fisherman, however, is not
necessarily a logician. He fishes, we will say, with a certain fly without
success; changes it for something different in colour, and lo! fish after
fish succumbs to its attractions. What more can he wish? Of course the fly
has done it.' It does not seem to occur to him that had he continued to fish
with No. 1 the result might, very possibly at least, have been the same. In
fact we have here simply the old problem of post hoc or Propter hoc.
As a rule gillies and boatmen
are very opinionative as to the patterns that alone, according to them, are
of any use in their particular waters, and for the sake of peace it is as
well, perhaps, to give in to their prejudices. Should the angler, however,
persist in the use of some unorthodox lure and achieve success, he must by
no means assume that he has thereby converted his attendant, who will
believe, and probably assert, that had his advice been followed the bag
would doubtless have been doubled.
It is not always so, however
; there are some in the ranks, more especially among the younger and better
educated, who have profited by experience and observation, as in the
following instance, which took place some years ago on a well-known stretch
of the Aberdeenshire Dee. Pool after pool had been fished in vain, and a new
cast having been reached the old vexed question was discussed with the
gillie, who on that famed water had assisted at the death of hundreds of
salmon.
`Shall we try Jock Scott?'
'Well it might do very well.'
`Or this Gordon ?'
`It's a capital fly the
Gordon.'
'Or a Thunder and Lightning?'
'Couldn't do better!'
`In fact, then, it doesn't
matter which ?'
'No' a !'
One of the most successful
salmon fishers of the last generation, as the result of the experience of a
long life-time on Tay and Tummel, latterly confined himself strictly to a
single pattern of salmon-fly, dressed by himself. It was a thin, light-blue
floss-silk body ribbed with tinsel, and a light wing of mixed fibres.
When we come to consider
trout and troutflies, the question is in one way different; for whatever
salmon-flies, so-called, may be supposed to represent, they certainly bear
no resemblance to flies; whereas trout-flies are presumed to be imitations
of natural insects, at least in so far as river-fishing is concerned. One
might therefore suppose that here, at least, the matter of colour must be
all important, always assuming that trout have the power of colour
discrimination; and in the view of the `Dry-fly purist,' as he loves to be
termed, it is taken for granted that it is so. One has only to study the
very interesting text-books of Mr. Halford, for instance, to find that the
most delicate shades of colour in silk, hackle or wing are strenuously
insisted on.
Let us look, however, for a
moment on the other side of the shield, and recall the most interesting and
practical experiments of Sir Herbert Maxwell, well known as an angling
expert, an observant naturalist, and one endowed to an unusual degree with
the faculty of critical analysis and deduction. The results of his
experiments are embodied in a report published in the Field of 19th June,
1897. As all readers may not have access to it, a short summary may be of
interest. Sir Herbert had some May-flies dressed in the ordinary way, except
that they were dyed of a brilliant scarlet or blue ; and with these he
proceeded to fish during the May-fly season in a well known southern trout
stream noted for its large and wary trout. Beginning with the usual more or
less exact copy of the natural insect, with which he caught several trout,
he then proceeded in bright sunshine to cast a scarlet fly over a rising
fish; it was at once accepted, and a fine trout landed-all doubtless to the
utter astonishment of the attendant keeper. The same fly was then presented
to another trout, with the same immediate result. A brilliant blue May-fly
was mounted and just as greedily taken by another victim ; and so on
throughout a most successful day, when some thirty trout, none under a pound
in weight, were landed, of which nine weighing 13 1/2 lbs. were kept. Of the
thirty a few were caught on the usual imitation ; the rest took the gaudy
scarlets and blues, some of which eccentric-looking patterns are now before
me. Sir Herbert then gives in detail a second and equally successful
experiment in another river, where trout up to 2 or 3
lbs. in weight fell victims to the same
unorthodox flies.'
Against this instance may be
given an experience of the writer's, which seems to point in the opposite
direction. Fishing a number of years ago on the River Deveron, a big rise of
fly came on suddenly, and large trout were at at once rising in a 'boil' in
all directions; but, as so often happens in such circumstances, they would
not look at the artificial flies offered to them. After several changes, a
cast of Yorkshire hackles was tried with gratifying success, and a nice
basket of good average size was secured. But every one of these was taken on
one particular fly, the so-called `partridge and blue.' This fly, moreover,
was not at the end of the cast of four flies-usually the most killing
position-but higher up.
This was thought at the moment to be a great
discovery, and many 'blue partridges' were sent for; but never again was it
found to be of much, or any use. Now all the other flies on the cast were
likewise Yorkshire hackles of exactly the same size and pattern, save only
colour, and all were patterns of proved value as killers on Deveron; so
wherefore the preference for that particular blue? To the writer the riddle
remains unsolved.
On the point in question, Mr.
J. A. HarvieBrown says:' 'Trout show decided preferences, for colours (at
least educated trout).' That writer's opinion on any subject connected with
Angling or Natural History must carry great weight, although in this
instance running counter to the experience of Sir Herbert Maxwell, as given
above.
To leave the Salmonidae for
the moment, and to turn to the pike, it seems to have been long generally
held that a `bit of red' about the bait often proved enticing. We have all
seen the tuft of red wool so frequently tied round the end triangle of a
spoon. In corroboration of this theory a friend recently related the
following instance to me. During a visit to Loch Awe, trout fishing being
'off,' it was resolved to make a raid in the loch upon the pike, of of which
there are only too many for the wellbeing of the nobler species. Two rods
were equipped, the one with an ordinary brown phantom, the other with a red
one-at one time, if memory serves, esteemed to be 'good medicine' for salmon
in Loch Tay.
The pike proved to be in
taking humour, and the result was something like nine or ten of good average
weight. But every one of these was caught on the scarlet bait ; the other,
with exactly the same chance, did not elicit so much as a casual `offer.' It
is, perhaps, worthy of notice that a pike following a spinning bait will see
it more or less on its own level and often against a background, say of
weeds or shore ; and this is also true to some extent in the case of the
salmon, which frequently follows the fly a long way before making the final
dash. Whereas, as a rule, the trout must see the fly against the sky and
light, presenting therefore only a dark outline.
The same friend was good
enough to call attention to a passage in Herbert Spencer's Autobiography,
bearing on the subject under consideration:
`My constitutional tendency
to call in question current opinions, was manifested when fishing, as on
other occasions. While in Wales the year before, occupied in writing on
Psychology and occasionally casting a fly over a stream or llyn, it occurred
to me that, considering how low is the nervous organization of fishes, it is
unlikely that they should be able to discriminate so nicely as the current
ideas respecting artificial flies imply-unlikely, too, that they should have
such erratic fancies as to be taken by combinations of differently coloured
feathers, like no living creature ever seen.
`I acted upon my scepticism,
and ignored the local traditions. Hearing me vent my heresies, the farmer,
tenant of Beoch, challenged me to a competition. It was scarcely a fair one,
for my flies, made by myself without practice, were, of course, ill-made,
and the bungling make of them introduced an irrelevant factor into the
competition. Notwithstanding this, however, fishing ,from the same boat we
came back ties ; showing that the local flies had no advantage.
`I may add here that in
subsequent years I systematically tested this current belief in local flies;
and on various lochs and four different rivers found it baseless. This
experience furnished me with a good illustration of the uncritical habits of
thought characteristic, not of the common people only, but of those who have
received University educations. For in every case I have found highly
cultivated men -professors and others such-accepting without hesitation the
dogmas of keepers and gillies concerning the flies of the river. Always
their assigned reason is that these dogmas express the results of
experience.
'But inquiry would show that
those who utter them have never established them by comparisons of numerical
results. They simply repeat, and act upon, what they have been told by their
predecessors; never dreaming of methodically testing their predecessors'
statements by trying, whether, all other things being equal, other colours
and mixtures of colours would not answer as well. The delusion results from
pursuing what, in inductive logic, is called the method of agreement, and
not checking its results by the method of difference.’
This passage emphasises
admirably the vague attitude of thought of even educated men on such
questions, and the fallacy of drawing general or universal conclusions from
isolated and exceptional instances. If we are to arrive at a more exact
apprehension of the powers and limits of the vision of fish, and of the
impressions produced in them by colours, we must first have a series of long
and carefully conducted experiments. The results, duly noted and tabulated,
would form a ground-work for investigations that should lead to conclusions
of greatest interest to anglers and naturalists. |